DYNOMIGHT ABOUT RSS SUBSTACK

It’s perfectly valid for a trait to be more than 100% heritable

It’s perfectly valid for a trait to be more than 100% heritable

Updated Aug 2022

All psychological traits are heritable. This is the best replicated finding in all of behavioral genetics. Some recent numbers include:

Religiosity 44%
Schizophrenia 79%
Big five personality traits ~40%

But what, exactly, does “heritability” mean?

I used to have a mental model something like this: Each person has some number of “religiosity points” that come from genes and some number that come from the environment. If religiosity was 40% genetic, I pictured there being 10 total points, 4 of which come from genes and 6 of which come from the environment:

 Genes       ★★★☆      
 Environment ★★☆☆☆☆    
 Total       ★★★★★☆☆☆☆☆


The problem with this picture—aside from being completely wrong—is that it suggests heritability is an immutable constant, like the number of chromosomes in a cell or the fine structure constant. This is totally incorrect.

So what is heritability? It’s the ratio of the genetic variance of a trait with the total variance, including all causes. This post will explore this definition. We’ll see that it leads to several puzzles:

  • How do changes in the environment lead to changes in heritability?
  • Why is heritability often higher for traits that seem less important? For example, why is pig back-fat thickness 14× more heritable than pig litter size?
  • How can a trait still be 100% heritable, even when there are large environmental effects?
  • How, when there are correlations between genes and the environment, can traits can be more than 100% heritable?

The only math we’ll use is the concept of variance. If you’re not familiar with that, just think of it as “how variable” something is. Humans have high variance in how much we like folk music, but low variance in our number of fingers.

One note on terminology: Biologists use “phenotype” to refer to what actually happens, including all genetic or environmental causes. The phenotypic length of your foot is, thrillingly, the actual length of your foot.

Simplest example

Say that people’s height depends on only one gene, which can be either short or tall. The genetic contribution to height is 2m if they get the short gene, and 4m if they get the tall gene. (The math is easier if we make everyone giants.)

genes Genetic value
short 2
tall 4

The only thing that matters in the environment is food. At birth, people are assigned either to diet or to feast. If on a diet, they lose 1m of height compared to their genetic baseline. If on a feast, they gain 1m of height.

env Environmental value
diet -1
feast +1

A person’s height is the sum of their genetic and environmental values. Using P for phenotypic, write the final height as

P(genes,env) = G(genes) + E(env).

With all these assumptions, can we calculate heritability? Not yet! We need the population distribution, i.e., what fraction of people have each combination of genes and environments. Assume for now that half of people have each gene, and half of people have each environment, and that these are independent.

population distribution diet feast
short ¼ ¼
tall ¼ ¼

The definition of heritability

Given any trait with P=G+E, the (broad-sense) heritability is

H2 = var(G) / var(P).

Heritability is how much the genetic contribution to the trait varies as compared to how much that trait varies overall. These variances are computed with respect to random people from the population.

In the above example, the variance of E is one, since it is -1 half the time, and +1 the other half. Similarly, G is always one off from its mean of three, so the variance of G is also one. Since the genes and the environment are independent, the variance of their sum is var(P)=var(G)+var(E). This tells us that

H2 = ½.

This is natural, since the effects of genes and environments are symmetric—changing either one changes height by 2m.

Let them eat more (or possibly less)

Let’s change how much food people get. Suppose that there’s some parameter b that reflects how much more food the feast group gets than the diet group.

env Environmental value
diet -b
feast +b

As b changes, the environmental variance changes. If you feed the two groups almost equally (b is near zero) then the variance is near zero. As the difference between groups increases, variance increases.

scenario 2 variances

Here’s what the corresponding heritability looks like:

scenario 2 heritability

If the two groups are fed equally, then height is 100% heritable. If the feast group is fed much more (b=2), then height is only 20% heritable.

Takeaway: Heritability depends on how variable the environment is. If the environment becomes more variable, heritability decreases. If the environment becomes more consistent, heritability increases.

Example: Changing the heritability of IQ

Research suggests that childhood lead exposure can decrease IQ by 5-10 points and heavy prenatal alcohol exposure can decrease IQ by 15-20 points. Current estimates are that intelligence is 40-80% heritable. But suppose society managed to eliminate lead and alcohol exposure. Environmental variance would decrease, and heritability would go up.

In theory, it’s possible to push the heritability of all traits to 100%. Just make sure each person is exposed to completely identical environments. (You can argue that free will or quantum mechanics or the butterfly effect means there is no such thing as two identical environments, but let’s not get distracted.)

One could decrease the heritability by doing the opposite: Take half of kids and make it illegal to teach them to read or whatever. Then, take the other half of kids and give them private tutors. Environmental variance would increase, and heritability would go down.

The purge

Now let’s delete a bunch of people. After the purge, instead of half being short and half tall, some fraction a are short and a fraction (1-a) are tall. If short people are more likely than tall people to survive the purge, then a > ½. If short people are less likely to survive, then a < ½.

Leave the food difference fixed at b=1. The environment doesn’t change during the purge, so the variance of E remains one.

The genetic variance does change. If almost everyone has short genes (a is near zero), then almost everyone will have G=2, meaning the variance of G will be small. If a is ½, we get the previous model. A bit of math gives the following plot.

scenario 1 variances

The heritability is still H2 = var(G) / (var(G)+var(E)), which now looks like this:

scenario 1 heritability

Heritability decreases hugely if the population gets really imbalanced in either direction.

Takeaway: Heritability depends on how much genetic diversity there is.

Example: Hair in Japan

How heritable is hair color in Japan, before anyone goes gray? Say 99% of people have genes for black hair, so var(G) is very low. Yet, lots of people have (artificially) non-black hair, so var(P) will be reasonably high. Assume that the choice to dye your hair isn’t genetically determined. Then, hair color in Japan has low heritability. (Lower, for example, than the US, which has more genetic diversity in hair color.) At the same time, we know that genes pretty much 100% determine what color hair grows on your head. Heritability is not a measure of how deterministic genes are in their effects.

Example: Heritability in different species

Here are some numbers, courtesy of Falconer’s Introduction to Quantitative Genetics.

Animal Trait Heritability (%)
Human Height 65
  Immunoglobulin 45
Cow Adult weight 65
  Butterfat % 40
  Milk yield 35
Pig Back-fat thickness 70
  Efficiency of food conversion 50
  Litter size 5
Chicken Weight 55
  Egg production 10
Mouse Tail length 40
  Weight 35
  Litter size 20
Fruit Fly Bristle number 50
  Body size 40
  Ovary size 30
  Egg production 20

Notice anything? The closer something is to reproduction, the lower heritability is. This seems odd—isn’t the number of eggs a fly produces incredibly important? Yes, but that’s precisely the point! Because it is so important, evolution strongly selects for the genes with the optimal egg production numbers. This leaves little genetic variance, which drives heritability down.

So low heritability doesn’t mean that genes don’t matter — it just means that in the current population, most people have genes that do similar stuff, so observed differences are mostly due to environmental causes.

(Technically, the numbers in the above table are estimates of narrow-sense heritability rather than broad-sense heritability as we are discussing. But this doesn’t matter since we’re only looking at the general trend.)

Selective feeding

So far, genes and the environment have been independent. No one looked at your genes when deciding if you get a diet or feast environment. But they could! What would happen then?

Let’s assume genes and the environment have the same effects as in the simplest model, i.e. b = 1 and a = ½. Now, while half of people are short/tall, and half get a diet/feast environment, let’s change the odds that feasts go to the people with short vs. tall genes. Specifically, take this distribution:

population distribution diet feast
short ¼ - c ¼ + c
tall ¼ + c ¼ - c

Here, c is any number between -¼ and +¼. If c = -¼, then people with short genes always get a diet, and people with tall genes always get a feast. If c = +¼, it’s the opposite.

Now, there’s something odd about this scenario. The genetic and environmental variances are always one. That’s because, no matter the value of c, the distribution over genes and environments are always uniform. (You can check that the row and column sums in the above table are always 0.5.)

However, the phenotypic variance does change with c. Suppose that c = ¼. People with short genes always get a feast, and thus have a height of 3. People with tall genes always get a diet, and so also have a height of 3. Everyone has the same height, so the phenotypic variance is zero!

Or suppose that c = -¼. People with short genes always get a diet, and so have a height of 1. People with tall genes always get a feast, and so have a height of 5. There is lots of phenotypic variance.

scenario 3 variances

(Mathematically, what’s happening here is that G and E are no-longer independent, so the variance of P is not just the sum of the variances of G and E but must be adjusted to include their (now-nonzero) covariance.)

The heritability, as ever, is H2=var(G)/var(P). If we plot this it looks like the following:

scenario 3 heritability

In the “starve the short” regime (negative c) heritability decreases. In the “feed the short” regime (positive c) it increases.

Speaking of which, on the right of the plot, heritability goes above one! This is not a mistake. Remember, heritability is the ratio of genetic variance and phenotypic variance. Usually, the environment creates more variance, so this ratio is less than one. However, if people with short genes get more food than people with tall genes, then environment decreases variance. (Technically, if c=¼, then the phenotypic variance is zero, meaning the heritability is infinite!)

Takeaway: When the environment depends on genes, things get weird. If the environment exacerbates genetic differences, that decreases heritability. If the environment reduces genetic differences, then that increases heritability.

Example: Education and intelligence

It’s plausible that education exacerbates genetic differences in intelligence. In most places today, I’d bet that genetic intelligence is positively correlated with the quality and quantity of education someone gets. (Partly because intelligence is correlated with income and partly because it helps to qualify for scholarships and selective schools/classes.) It also appears that education increases intelligence.

Now, imagine that society is reformed to remove correlations between genetic intelligence and education. (Maybe all students are assigned to public schools at random.) This would be like moving to the right on the above graph. Thus, strangely, more equal access to education would cause heritability to go up. If we went further and provided better education to less gifted students, that would increase things even further.

It doesn’t matter if more equal education is a good idea, or even if we truly are in a “starve the short” regime now. The point is that heritability depends on society.

Three key takeaways

  1. Less random environments lead to higher heritability. If everyone had completely equal environments, every trait would be 100% heritable.

  2. Heritability depends on the amount of genetic variance in the population. If people have similar genes, heritability goes down. You don’t need anything dramatic like new mutations.

  3. If genetics are correlated with the environment, weird things can happen. If environmental correlations decrease variance, then total variance might be lower then genetic variance. If so, heritability is greater than 100%!

Heritability is really just a ratio of variances. You’ll mislead yourself thinking about it any other way.

new dynomight every thursday
except when not

(or try substack or rss) ×
Please show lots of digits

as many as you're allowed to give me

Hi there. It’s me, the person who stares very hard at the numbers in the papers you write. I’ve brought you here today to ask a favor. Say you wrote something like this: There were 446 students tested. The left-handed...

Arithmetic is an underrated world-modeling technology

if you keep units

Of all the cognitive tools our ancestors left us, what’s best? Society seems to think pretty highly of arithmetic. It’s one of the first things we learn as children. So I think it’s weird that only a tiny percentage of...

Datasets that change the odds you exist

Stats for dangerous situations

It's October 1962. The Cuban missile crisis just happened, thankfully without apocalyptic nuclear war. But still: Apocalyptic nuclear war easily could have happened. Crises as serious as the Cuban missile crisis clearly aren't *that* rare, since one just happened. You...

Using axis lines for good or evil

add them only if they mean something

Say you want to plot some data. You could just plot it by itself. Or you could put lines on the left and bottom. Or you could put lines everywhere. Or you could be weird. Which is right? Many people...

Prediction market does not imply causation

Unless you're careful, conditional prediction markets have all the same problems as observational studies.

We all want to make good decisions. But it’s hard because we aren’t sure what’s going to happen. Like, say you want to know if CO₂ emissions will go up in 10 years. One of our best ideas is to...

The conspiratorial Monty Hall problem

What if you and Monty decide to cheat?

The Monty Hall problem has now been a pox on humanity for two generations, diverting perfectly good brains away from productive uses. Hoping to exacerbate this problem, some time ago I announced a new and more pernicious variant: What if...

Why ‟controlling for a variable” doesn't (usually) work

It's just adding a variable to a regression.

I’ve always seen cathedrals as presenting a kind of implicit argument to atheists. Something like: God must exist, because otherwise it would have been insane for people to build this:

Social dynamics of bluetooth speakers

A mathematical model of who turns on their bluetooth speaker at the beach.

Say you're at a park or a beach. How many people will have bluetooth speakers on? It seems to me there are three types of people: The main characters always turn on their speakers regardless of what anyone else is...

Simpson's paradox all the way down

Visualizes Simpson's paradox, and shows how it's a deeper problem than many people realize.

It's hard to get into Oxford. Is it easier if your parents are rich? In 2013, The Guardian showed noticed something disturbing: Students from (expensive) independent schools were accepted more often that students from state schools (28% vs 20%). Of...

The simplest possible way to convert Celsius and Fahrenheit

If you can switch the order of two numbers, you can convert temperatures.

This is a new way to convert temperatures between Celsius and Fahrenheit. It’s not the most accurate method, but it’s surely the easiest.

Making the Monty Hall problem weirder but obvious

It's this simple: Do you want what's behind one door or the other nine?

Here’s an Obvious Problem: There are 10 doors. A car is behind a random door, goats behind the others. Do you want what’s behind door 1, or what’s behind all the other doors? That’s easy, right? Well, how about the...